Группа авторов

The Science of Reading


Скачать книгу

the chapters in Parts 1 and 2 highlights new findings and research directions. A number of themes emerge, not least a greater recognition of the need to unite theories about development and learning with theories about skilled processing – after all, reading in proficient adults is the product of many years of reading experience that has optimized the reading system to the task in hand. Another important development has been to better understand how morphological structure is represented in written language, and how this is accessed and used during word recognition. Like the mappings between orthography and phonology, morphological information is systematically represented in orthography in ways that reflect graded regularities. Learning to read efficiently requires children to develop a reading system that adequately embodies the distributional information coded by the writing system. In line with this view, several chapters emphasize statistical learning approaches that view skilled reading as the emergent product of interactions between readers and texts.

      Part 4 turns to reading in different languages, a field that has burgeoned in recent years. While the science of reading is still dominated by studies in English, reading scientists agree that theories must pay attention to the universal features of reading as well as those that develop from specific experiences with particular writing systems. Here, chapters provide overviews of alphabetic, alpha‐syllabic, and Chinese orthographies, and elsewhere in the book, reference is made to research emanating from different writing systems.

      Part 5 considers reading disorders. As in the first edition, there are chapters on both developmental and acquired disorders of reading. In the current volume, there is more convergence between these two strands of research, both noting that selective deficits are rare in developmental as well as in acquired dyslexic populations. Importantly, there is an intimate relationship between reading and language as seen from the perspective of learning to read with a language or hearing impairment as well as in some acquired dyslexias. New to this model is consideration of cognitive models of comorbidity among dyslexia, mathematics disorder, and ADHD, underlining the interconnections between the cognitive and brain systems that underpin reading.

      Finally, Part 6 is concerned with aetiology and considers both the biological and social factors that are associated with learning to read. The chapters take us from molecular genetics to the environment provided by parents, classrooms and neighborhoods. They remind us that reading is complex and that to understand it we need to consider genetic influences and their interaction with multiple environmental factors during development. It closes with a detailed review of the neural bases of reading and its development. Neuroimaging methods have expanded since the first edition of the handbook and an important development has been consideration of neuroimaging data in the context of both cognitive and developmental theories.

      As this second edition is published, it is tempting to forecast what developments might feature in a hypothetical third edition. Reading and writing go hand in hand, but it remains the case that scientific studies of reading are far more common than scientific studies of writing, and those studies of writing that exist focus heavily on spelling and much less so on sentence or discourse level writing. Learning to be literate demands that children write effectively as well as read, and there are theoretical reasons to consider the production demands of writing as critical to comprehension. We hope in the coming years that there will be more work on the science of writing and that this will be integrated with studies of reading.

      We also predict more research that connects issues in learning to read with those studied in the context of skilled reading, both in terms of word reading and comprehension processes. There is certainly room for more theoretical connection, and for methodological and statistical approaches to be shared, including the exciting developments that will enhance our understanding of the genetic and neural underpinnings of reading. Individual differences in reading are already well‐represented in this edition. Nevertheless, important questions remain, not least when considering the role of linguistic, cultural, and contextual influences in the home and school environment. We also predict greater awareness of individual differences in adults.

      Finally, as research connects across the lifespan, we might also consider the fate of people who leave school with low levels of literacy. Even in high‐income countries, approximately 20% of 15‐year‐olds do not attain the levels of literacy needed to allow them to participate effectively in life. These individuals are underrepresented in scientific studies of reading, and arguably too in terms of evidence‐informed adult education and support in the community.

      Maggie Snowling, Charles Hulme, Kate Nation

      2021

      We thank all of the authors of this handbook who have given selflessly of their time to complete these chapters during the COVD‐19 pandemic. We have learned a great deal from the experience of editing and discussing their contributions. We thank Jenny Diment and Rhianna Watt for their invaluable support at all stages of the editorial process.

      Word recognition is the foundation of reading; all other processes are dependent on it. If word recognition processes do not operate fluently and efficiently, reading will be at best highly inefficient. The study of word recognition processes is one of the oldest areas of research in the whole of experimental psychology (Cattell, 1886). In the opening chapter of this second edition of The Science of Reading, Perfetti and Helder provide a comprehensive overview of recent advances in the study of reading, with particular emphasis on skilled reading in adults. This chapter sets the stage for the chapters that follow. Guided by the analogy of reading as a rapidly moving stream, they use the Reading Systems Framework to show how different knowledge sources, including critical lexical knowledge, underpin the processes that take the reader from word identification to comprehension (and how these may be disrupted in reading disorders). In so doing they consider the range of methodologies that have been used to investigate reading processes, including neurophysiological measures and brain imaging, emphasizing that computational methods and language corpora together add precision to theoretical proposals.