K., & Davis, M. H. (2008). Morphological decomposition based on the analysis of orthography. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23(7–8), 942–971. doi: 10.1080/01690960802069730.
58 Rastle, K., Davis, M. H., Marslen‐Wilson, W. D., & Tyler, L. K. (2000). Morphological and semantic effects in visual word recognition: A time‐course study. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15(4–5), 507–537. doi: 10.1080/01690960050119689.
59 Rastle, K., Davis, M. H., & New, B. (2004). The broth in my brother’s brothel: Morpho‐orthographic segmentation in visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(6), 1090–1098. doi: 10.3758/BF03196742.
60 Rayner, K., Sereno, S. C., Lesch, M. F., & Pollatsek, A. (1995). Phonological codes are automatically activated during reading: Evidence from an eye movement priming paradigm. Psychological Science, 6(1), 26–32. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‐9280.1995.tb00300.x.
61 Rueckl, J. G., & Raveh, M. (1999). The influence of morphological regularities on the dynamics of a connectionist network. Brain and Language, 68(1–2), 110–117. doi: 10.1006/brln.1999.2106.
62 Schiff, R., Raveh, M., & Fighel, A. (2012). The development of the Hebrew mental lexicon: When morphological representations become devoid of their meaning. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16(5), 383–403. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2011.571327.
63 Seidenberg, M. S. (2011). Reading in different writing systems: One architecture, multiple solutions. In P. McCardle, B. Miller, J. R. Lee, & O. J. L. Tzeng (Eds.), The extraordinary brain series. Dyslexia across languages: Orthography and the brain–gene–behavior link (pp. 146–168). Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing.
64 Seymour, P. H., Aro, M., Erskine, J. M., & Collaboration with COST Action A8 Network. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94(2), 143–174. doi: 10.1348/000712603321661859.
65 Solomyak, O., & Marantz, A. (2010). Evidence for early morphological decomposition in visual word recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(9), 2042–2057. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21296.
66 Taft, M. (1979). Recognition of affixed words and the word frequency effect. Memory & Cognition, 7(4), 263–272. doi: 10.3758/BF03197599.
67 Taft, M. (1994). Interactive‐activation as a framework for understanding morphological processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 9(3), 271–294. doi: 10.1080/01690969408402120.
68 Taft, M., & Ardasinski, S. (2006). Obligatory decomposition in reading prefixed words. The Mental Lexicon, 1(2), 183–199. doi: 10.1075/ml.1.2.02taf.
69 Taft, M., & Forster, K. I. (1975). Lexical storage and retrieval of prefixed words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14(6), 638–647. doi: 10.1016/S0022‐5371(75)80051‐X.
70 Taft, M., & Nguyen‐Hoan, M. (2010). A sticky stick? The locus of morphological representation in the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25(2), 277–296. doi: 10.1080/01690960903043261.
71 Tamminen, J., Davis, M. H., & Rastle, K. (2015). From specific examples to general knowledge in language learning. Cognitive Psychology, 79, 1–39. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2015.03.003.
72 Taylor, J. S. H., Davis, M. H., & Rastle, K. (2019). Mapping visual symbols onto spoken language along the ventral visual stream. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(36), 17723–17728. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1818575116.
73 Taylor, J. S. H., Duff, F. J., Woollams, A. M., Monaghan, P., & Ricketts, J. (2015). How word meaning influences word reading. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(4), 322–328. doi: 10.1177/0963721415574980.
74 Taylor, J. S. H., Rastle, K., & Davis, M. H. (2013). Can cognitive models explain brain activation during word and pseudoword reading? A meta‐analysis of 36 neuroimaging studies. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 766–791. doi: 10.1037/a0030266.
75 Treiman, R., Wolter, S., & Kessler, B. (2020). How sensitive are adults to the role of morphology in spelling? Morphology. doi: 10.1007/s11525‐020‐09356‐4.
76 Ulicheva, A., Harvey, H., Aronoff, M., & Rastle, K. (2020). Skilled readers’ sensitivity to meaningful regularities in English writing. Cognition, 195, 103810. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.09.013.
77 van Orden, G. C. (1987). A ROWS is a ROSE: Spelling, sound, and reading. Memory & Cognition, 15(3), 181–198. doi: 10.3758/BF03197716.
78 Whiting, C., Shtyrov, Y., & Marslen‐Wilson, W. (2014). Real‐time functional architecture of visual word recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(2), 246–265. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00699.
79 Yablonski, M., Rastle, K., Taylor, J. S. H., & Ben‐Shachar, M. (2019). Structural properties of the ventral reading pathways are associated with morphological processing in adult English readers. Cortex, 116, 268–285. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.06.011.
Note
1 1 I say “largely” because there are inconsistencies in the relationship between spelling and sound (e.g., phone, tone, cone vs. done), and there are regularities in the relationship between spelling and meaning (e.g., snore, snarl, sneeze, snort all relate to the nose).
PART II Learning to Read and Spell
Most children are already competent users of their native language by the time they go to school, and reading develops from this foundation. Indeed, as Mattingly (1972) proposed more than 30 years ago, “reading is parasitic on speech.” However learning to read is not a straightforward matter because, at a minimum, it involves breaking a code that maps spoken language on to written language. How difficult it is to break the code, and how much else there is to learn before reaching an adult level of proficiency will depend on a wide range of factors, some intrinsic and others extrinsic to the child, including the language of learning. In the first edition of this book, these themes were developed in the chapter by Byrne (2005) who proposed that, at first, the child approaches reading with the incorrect hypothesis about what print represents. As a consequence, what needs to be taught is what the child does not bring to the learning situation. Focusing on learning to read in English, he argued that it is crucial for children to gain insight into the alphabetic principle – to acquire knowledge of letter‐sound mappings, understand that not all features of the sound stream are represented in the orthography, and that there are one‐to‐many and many‐to‐one correspondences between orthography and phonology. Treiman and Kessler (2005) discussed similar themes in their chapter focusing on the principles children need to understand when learning to write words, and the difficulties these pose. They also consider how the ease of learning to spell depends on the orthography children are learning. It is difficult to segment the speech stream into phonemic units, and therefore alphabetic systems are hard to learn; children with better phoneme awareness do better in this regard and letter knowledge can bootstrap the process.
In this second edition, Hamilton and Hayiou‐Thomas take a broader view of the skills that children bring to the task of learning to read and are required to secure a strong foundation. Emphasizing the importance of the quality (and not just quantity) of oral language input in the pre‐school years, they review evidence on how the home literacy environment prepares children for reading and how variation in this environment can limit as well as foster their success, touching on similar issues to those explored by Phillips and Lonigan (2005). The roles of factors that are extrinsic and intrinsic to the child (including genetic factors that place them at risk of reading or language problems) are considered drivers of learning and Hamilton and Hayiou‐Thomas emphasize a “division of labour” between the learner and their environment (Byrne, 1998). Their over‐arching message is that learning to read builds on language skills, honed through interactions among the child, the caregiver, and the book.
Castles and Nation take up the issue of how, after taking the first steps into literacy, children