clinical care at least half time. Since this study was part of a larger international study concerning the management of T2DM in different countries (health care systems) it was not possible to include international medical graduates ( IMGs ). We stratified physician subjects according to gender and level of clinical experience (graduated from medical school between 1993–1999 (less experience) or between 1969–1983 (more experience)) (there were 2 × 2 = 4 strata) and recruited eligible physicians until each of the 4 strata was complete. Each of 24 vignette pairs was viewed twice in each of the 4 strata for a total of (24 × 2 × 4=) 192 physicians.
Sampling of physicians was done using a purposive sample to fill each design cells (male/female; more/less experienced) – rather than a straight random sample of physicians. In this way, they were more quickly able to get the required numbers to fit the profiles they needed for the study.
Thus, the permutations were as shown in Table 3.1:
Their results showed that doctors were significantly more likely to diagnose diabetes in the black and the Hispanic ‘patients’, 73.4% of the physicians' diagnosed T2DM when the ‘patient’ was black, 60.9% when Hispanic and 48.4% when white (p = 0.009) – when fully adjusted for the ‘patient's’ age, gender, and SES, the percentage of physicians giving a diabetes diagnosis was 64.5% for lower SES patients (janitor) versus 57.3% for upper SES patients (lawyer) (p = 0.265) and the corresponding level of certainty for lower SES patients was 24.4 compared to 20.4 for upper SES patients (p = 0.241). In other words, in making an initial diagnosis, physicians focus more on the ‘patient's’ race/ethnicity rather than their SES.
They concluded: ‘this paper suggests that the signs and symptoms of T2DM, when undiagnosed in the general community, are patterned by SES and not race/ethnicity and that following diagnosis by a physician they are patterned by race/ethnicity’.
Don't you think that is interesting? It raised, for them, the question about whether the higher rates of black and Hispanic persons diagnosed with T2DM was a result of a racially‐skewed stereotype rather than a true profile of the population in need of care.
Remember Mick Bloor's saying: ‘Research is the art of the possible’? John's study was well‐funded. Thus, they were able to hire and train actresses. They were able to develop the scenarios of the ‘patients’. They were able to conduct the sampling in a very professional manner. They were able to get the physicians to spend their time taking part in the experiment. Thus, they were able to go beyond the laboratory. They were able to do the experiment with real, practising physicians, rather than with medical students. And they had the expert statistical expertise required for their very complex analyses within their organisation. Well done them!
Example 5: Reduction of Postoperative Pain by Encouragement and Instruction of Patients
This is one of my favourite field experiments. It is halfway between being an experiment and a randomised control trial (RCT) (see next chapter). It was conducted in 1964, so it is a classic, both in the temporal (time) sense and in its importance. A group of anaesthetists and surgeons in Boston (USA) were concerned about the problems of treating postoperative pain (Egbert et al. 1964). So, they devised a study to test a theory about how postop pain might be better managed.
TABLE 3.1 Patient and physician characteristics in each vignette.
‘Patient’ Characteristics in each Vignette | |||||||||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Male | Female | ||||||||||||||||||||||
Age 35 | Age 65 | Age 35 | Age 65 | ||||||||||||||||||||
Low SES | High SES | Low SES | High SES | Low SES | High SES | Low SES | High SES | ||||||||||||||||
B | H | W | B | H | W | B | H | W | B | H | W | B | H | W | B | H | W | B | H | W | B | H | W |
Less Experienced | More Experienced | Less Experienced | More Experienced | ||||||||||||||||||||
Male | Female | ||||||||||||||||||||||
Physician Characteristics |
They recruited 97 patients who were admitted to their hospital for abdominal operations (appendicectomy, cholycystectomy, etc.). These were randomly allocated to a ‘special care’ (experimental) group or a ‘normal care’ (control) group. The control group all received their normal treatment – no more and no less. Throughout their hospital stay.
Those in the special care group received their normal care, plus:
1 When the anaesthetist visited them the evening before the operation, they were told that they would experience pain after the operation, how severe it would be, and how long it would last. They were reassured that this pain was normal after an operation.
2 They were told that they would receive pain killing drugs.
3 They were given advice about how to minimise the pain – by relaxing the abdominal muscles. They were given instructions on how to turn over in bed, and they were told that they should request medication if they needed it.
4 On the afternoon, after the operation, the anaesthetist visited them again. He went over what he had told them the evening before; reassured them that their pain was normal; and they were again told to ask for medication if they wanted it.
5 This was repeated the next morning, and once or twice a day until they did not need any more analgesic medication.
The control group received their normal care throughout.
An independent observer (an anaesthetist who the patients had not met) visited 57 of them to record the patients' evaluations about their pain, as well as noting his own impressions about their appearance.
This was a double‐blind experiment. The patients were not told that they were part of an experiment – first blind. The nursing staff, who administered the medications, and the surgical staff who made decisions about the patients' discharge, did not know that there was an experiment in progress – second blind. In this way, they avoided the… what? That's right … the Hawthorne Effect, or in this case, the placebo effect. The researchers were well aware of the potential for a placebo effect – enhanced by patients trying to please them and nursing staff trying to please the surgeons – or vice versa. So by using