David Bowers

Understanding Clinical Papers


Скачать книгу

An illustration of prevalence of a clinical feature, determined in a cross-sectional study.

      Source: From Culbertson et al. (2004), © 2004, Elsevier.

An illustration of extract from a cross-sectional (incidence) study about frequency of stroke.

      Source: From Wolfe et al. (2002), © 2002, BMJ Publishing Group Ltd.

An illustration of the use of a survey method in a cross-sectional study.

      Source: From Ellenbecker et al. (2004), © 2004, Elsevier.

      When researchers study a group of subjects over time in a longitudinal study, there is more research work to be done than in a cross‐sectional study; subjects must be followed up one or more times to determine their prognosis or outcome.

      The kinds of observational studies we've seen above are among the simplest form of clinical research. In the next chapter, more complex observational studies are described – quantitative analytic studies. What they have in common (and in this way they differ from the above study types) is that they generally involve comparison of two or more groups of people and often attempt to infer something about cause of symptoms or conditions.

      Remember from the previous chapter that, compared with descriptive studies of a single group, analytic studies are more complex (and often more interesting). Analytic studies will usually involve some comparison and frequently aim to elucidate cause and effect in some way. Four kinds of observational analytic study will be described here:

       Ecological studies

       Cross‐sectional, two‐group studies

       Case–control studies

       Cohort analytic studies

      A neat way of tackling questions about the cause of disease or other health events is to sit in a library (or, more likely, at a computer), locate routinely collected data, and put population data about disease frequency (e.g. regional deaths from lung cancer) together with data about exposure to a risk (e.g. regional data on tobacco consumption). By so doing, you might find that regions with high lung cancer death rates were also the ones with high tobacco consumption. Suppose also that you found the corollary – that low mortality areas were associated with low tobacco consumption – then your findings would support a link between the supposed risk (smoking) and the target disorder (lung cancer).

Schematic illustration of the findings from an ecological study about smoking and domestic fires.

      Source: From Diekman et al. (2008), © 2008, BMJ Publishing Group Ltd.

      But you may by now have spotted a flaw in this type of study: we don't know whether the individual people who died in house fires were smokers. Put another way, it is possible for a study of this design to come up with these findings even if every person who died in a house fire was a non‐smoker. This flaw is sometimes called the ecological fallacy and is a consequence of the use of aggregated data rather than the more usual research method of collecting data for each individual study participant. The other three types of analytic study set out below are more satisfactory approaches to cause‐and‐effect questions because they are able to relate the supposed risk factor directly to the outcome in each study participant.

      Some cross‐sectional studies aim to shed light on cause and effect by recording whether people with a disease were more likely than people without the same disease to have experienced exposure to a risk factor. For example, for more than half a century researchers have recognized that patients with schizophrenia,