Anthony Ryle

Introducing Cognitive Analytic Therapy


Скачать книгу

self with group analysts. Foulkes, for example, saw individuals in a social fashion as being nodes in a “social matrix” (Foulkes & Anthony, 1957). Later group analytic writers (e.g., Brown & Zinkin, 1994; Pines, 1996) have also developed an interest in the (Bakhtinian) dialogic aspects of the self.

      Although object‐relations theorists were a major influence on the development of CAT, they were on the whole little interested in the concept of self. However, the current CAT model is close to some later authors in the object relations tradition, such as Sutherland (1980), Ogden (1990), and Sandler and Sandler (1998). It is also close to Bowlby's attachment theory (1988) and to subsequent work influenced by this (Fonagy & Target, 1997; and see Bateman, Brown, & Pedder, 2000) in their descriptions of the important role of internalization, and there are some similarities with more recent developments in cognitive psychology stressing the importance early interpersonal experience and of attachment patterns (see Gumley & Schwannauer, 2006; Guidano, 1987, 1991; Liotti, Cortina, & Farina, 2008; Neisser, 1993; Perris, 2000; Young, Klosko, & Weishaar, 2003). However, these authors do not take the further, and, in our view, important, conceptual leap of seeing the self as being essentially constituted by early, socially meaningful, sign‐mediated interpersonal experiences, as opposed to “representing” them mentally.

      Nonetheless, object‐relations theories made a considerable contribution by indicating the importance of early development in determining personality, by offering an account of how parental figures were “internalized” to form a part of the personality, and by recognizing the parallel, linked features of intrapsychic and interpersonal processes and their emergence in transference relationships (see also discussion in Ryle, 1996, 1997c). However, the attribution of these phenomena by some psychoanalytic object‐relations theorists to “splitting” and “projective identification,” and the location of “psychopathology” in the posited internal system of object relations and to “ego weakness,” innate destructive instinctual forces, and unconscious fantasy is seen as unhelpful and untenable from a CAT perspective. The phenomenon of projective identification, whereby others are powerfully induced to experience feelings or play roles which the person cannot tolerate, is not regarded as a defense in CAT; it is seen to represent an exaggerated example of the normal empathic processes of RR relating. CAT understands Self states (dissociated RRs) to be precarious and to leading the person to induce powerful identifying responses in, or to forcefully seek reciprocations from, others even where these are harmful. While usually discussed in relation to destructive procedures, CAT would understand that the same intense pressures can characterize the seeking for “good enough” or ideal care.

      Cognitive Psychology and Cognitive Therapy

      Later developments in cognitive‐behavior therapy (CBT), for example the work of Guidano (1987) or Neisser (1993) focusing on a concept of self, and those stressing the importance of early interpersonal experience and of attachment patterns, including in relation to dissociation (see Gumley & Schwannauer, 2006; Liotti et al., 2008; Perris, 2000), or interpersonally‐derived “schemas” (Young et al., 2003), or even those seeing the self as a set of information processing systems (Prescott, 2015), show some convergences with CAT. This would include clinical work noting and addressing ways in which so‐called “therapy interfering behaviors” or “resistance” may arise (Leahy, 2001). These have to some extent shifted attention to higher level functions and more complex disorders but important differences remain, as will become clear in later chapters. Notably, such approaches still lack, in our view, a clear and coherent means of understanding and effectively addressing “therapy interfering behaviors,” “resistance,” or therapeutic challenges and ruptures in a productive and non‐blaming way (see also Chapter 9).

      The early CAT model (PSM) therefore resembled cognitive ones but differed essentially in that the unit of observation—the procedural sequence—involved linking together environmental, mental, and behavioral phenomena and their implicit underlying relational origins. The level of address in CAT is on whole‐person Self processes and structures understood in developmental and contextual terms, whereas CBT remains usually focused on particular beliefs, symptoms, or behaviors and pays little attention to development or structure, or to socio‐cultural context. Some important differences deriving from these and as manifest in the clinical practice of CBT and CAT are further considered in Chapter 9.