too. Burton Blatt’s work exposed the myths of mental retardation, and my own earlier inquiries into that topic benefitted especially from the works (and kind encouragement) of Paul Lombardo and Alexander Tymchuk and the counsel of Fred Rich. My more recent efforts to sift through the primary literature were assisted by a number of doctoral students in psychology at Widener University: at various times, I have benefitted from the research assistance of Ann Christie, Leonard Goldschmidt, and David Nickelsen. Professor Susan Goldberg of Widener University and Professor Martin Levit at the University of Missouri-Kansas City reviewed drafts of selected chapters in an effort to make sure that the science was not obviously wrong; they deserve, of course, no blame for any lingering errors.
For the history too I have benefitted from the efforts of others. The works of Ira Berlin, Carl Degler, Eric Foner, John Hope Franklin, Leon Litwack, James McPherson, Edmund Morgan, and Winton Solberg figure prominently in the text, though again, as the notes reveal, there are many others. Peter Parish’s historiography of slavery, Alden Vaughn’s historiography of the roots of racism, and Eric Foner’s historiographical essay on Reconstruction were particularly valuable to this novice effort. On a more personal note, I must thank William Garfield for teaching me how to learn and appreciate history; I am grateful as well to the History Department at Davidson College for requiring all history majors to take a course in historiography (I am grateful too that they allowed me to graduate, despite an abysmally low History GRE score).
I was more at home with the legal analysis, but here too I must acknowledge certain debts. A. Leon Higginbotham and Thomas Morris have produced wonderful surveys of the law of slavery, and a loose coalition of contemporary legal scholars—the Critical Race Theorists—has generated invaluable critiques of the American law of “race.” The works of Derrick Bell, Richard Delgado, Charles Lawrence, and Patricia Williams figure prominently in the text; so too does the work of Martha Minow, on the construction of the many forms of “difference.” Alfred Avins’s partial collection of the debates over the Reconstruction Amendments provided a useful starting point for research, though I am certain that he would not approve of the way this text builds on his effort. Charles DiMaria, Hamel Vyas, Pamela Krauss, and Aaron Goldstein provided terrific research assistance; the last deserves special thanks for his help in sifting through the microfiche records of the congressional Reconstruction debates. Thanks too to Barbara Carcanague and Noreen McGlinchey of Widener University, and Despina Gimbel and Elyse Strongin at New York University Press, who provided outstanding administrative and technical support to the project. The Widener University School of Law provided a research stipend to fund part of this effort; special thanks to Dean Arthur N. Frakt for his generosity and support.
My more personal debts run the gamut, I suppose, from the ridiculous to the sublime. Close to the former, I feel obliged to acknowledge the constant companionship of Louis Armstrong, Ella Fitzgerald, Louis Jordan, and Bruce Springsteen, who were not, in truth, actually with me, but whose music was a steady inspiration as I pored through the notes and typed up the words. I would also like to thank the folks who populate the personal stories in this text. Some of them would recognize the events but not their names: I changed the names whenever I thought privacy was at issue and accuracy was not. The events, I think, mostly happened as I described them, though I have already discovered that recollections differ on some of the details. But the stories, in any event, are offered as “stories,” and their truths are decidedly personal.
Very much at the sublime end of the spectrum, I would like to thank the following family members, not only for giving me most of the stories, but also for their assistance, their support, their tolerance, and their affection: Patricia Eakin, Albert Griffiths, Alma Griffiths, Cynthia Hayman, Tom Hayman, Stephen Heim, Faye Kaufman, Galen Kaufman, Bob Maxwell, Harriet Maxwell, June Pesikey, William Sheridan, Katherine Thomas, Morris Thomas, Ron Whitehorne, and especially my mother, Norine Sheridan. The book is dedicated to them—but really, it is their book anyhow.
Special thanks as well to Niko Pfund at New York University Press for his remarkable patience, steady encouragement, and valuable insights. The same to Richard Delgado and Jean Stefancic, who helped inspire this effort and keep it on track; it would not have happened without them. And the most special of these special thanks to my frequent coauthor and always friend Nancy Levit; I suspected that it might be a real strain to write something without her, and am glad that I didn’t have to find out.
Finally, thanks to Alice Eakin, my editor, best friend, and wife. She provided the words when I needed them, and the ideas, and the muse—-and a whole world of kindness.
1 Introduction Smart People
I’m not sure when I found out that some kids had high IQs. When I did find out, I’m not sure I much cared. When we were kids, we had our own ideas about “smart,” and they had very little to do with IQs. The third-grade boys, for example, had developed their own distinct intellectual hierarchy: it consisted in small part of baseball trivia, in small part of the aptitude for petty crime, and in very substantial part of the skills—cognitive and otherwise—needed for insulting our peers (and, of course, their families). The girls, meanwhile, probably had their own hierarchy, but in the third grade, that was a mystery we boys had no interest in solving.
In the three-part hierarchy in which the boys subsisted, the ability to insult was undeniably the most important branch of intellect. It was also the most elaborate, itself consisting of three developmental stages: the first came with the recognition that curse words could be used as insults; the second was marked by the ability to use some curse words (one in particular) as participles to modify other curse words; and the third arrived with the realization that almost any curse word could be made doubly insulting by adding -face, -head, or -breath as a suffix. Progress through these stages, it seems to me now, was as much art as science: I remember one poor kid whose social fate was sealed the day he called me a “f—ing ass-head.”
There was one insult we used quite a bit, and it was about the only time we showed any interest in the IQ concept. For no specific reason, or at least not for reasons having anything to do with perceptions of intelligence, we found it immensely gratifying to call one another “retards.” We had, of course, no idea who or what a “retard” was, and we were fairly liberal in constructing synonyms: “reject” was thought to convey the same message, as were the more elaborate “mental retard,” “mental reject,” or, less elaborately, “mental.” All we knew about any of these terms was that they had all the ingredients for a good insult: they were apparently somehow demeaning; they had quite a funny sound to them; and no one, as far as we knew, would ever confront us with the embarrassing revelation that what we intended as an insult was in fact an accurate description.
All of this changed sometime in the third grade, when we discovered Mrs. Sweeney’s “special” class. We had wondered for some time why the window in Mrs. Sweeney’s door was covered with cardboard, wondered specifically why we kids weren’t supposed to look in. I suppose it never occurred to us that the cardboard also kept the kids inside from looking out, but then, lots of that kind of stuff never occurred to us. What did occur to us was that Mrs. Sweeney’s kids had to be “special” in some very strange way, strange enough that we had to be prevented from seeing them. Our imaginations ran wild with the possibilities, and we were not at all disappointed the day Dicky Hollins told us that he knew the secret to those kids, that his mom knew the mother of some kid in Mrs. Sweeney’s class, and the kid was, honest-to-god, a retard.
Just what that meant remained a mystery. For all we knew, “retards” were circus freaks or juvenile delinquents or some barely imaginable combination of the two. We deduced that they must be somehow pathetic and perhaps somehow frightening; we knew for sure that they were different from other kids, and that the difference was wildly fascinating.
For months, our school days were preoccupied with the effort to catch a glimpse of the retards. We’d linger outside Mrs. Sweeney’s door at lunch time, knock on her door and hide just around the corner, we’d come to school early in the hopes of seeing the retards arrive and stay late to catch them leaving, and through it all, we never saw more than Mrs. Sweeney’s disapproving frown. And then Mrs. Sweeney failed to show up for school one morning, and we were sure it was because the retards had