Группа авторов

Complexity Perspectives on Researching Language Learner and Teacher Psychology


Скачать книгу

and Teacher-Student Motivational Synergy: A Narrative of Language Teaching. London: Routledge.

      Poupore, G. (2018) A complex systems investigation of group work dynamics in L2 interactive tasks. Modern Language Journal 102 (2), 350–370.

      Richardson, K. and Cilliers, P. (2001) Special editors’ introduction: What is complexity science? A view from different directions. Emergence 3 (1), 5–23.

      Sade, L.A. (2011) Emerging selves, language learning and motivation through the lens of chaos. In G. Murray, X. Gao and M. Lamb (eds) Identity, Motivation and Autonomy in Language Learning (pp. 42–56). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.

      Sampson, R.J. (2015) Tracing motivational emergence in a classroom language learning project. System 50, 10–20.

      Sampson, R.J. (2016a) Complexity in Classroom Foreign Language Learning Motivation: A Practitioner Perspective from Japan. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.

      Sampson, R.J. (2016b) EFL teacher motivation in-situ: Co-adaptive processes, openness and relational motivation over interacting timescales. Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching 6 (2), 293–318.

      Sampson, R.J. (2018) Complexity in acting on images of ideal classmates in the L2 classroom. Konin Language Studies 6 (4), 387–410.

      Sampson, R.J. (2019) Real people with real experiences: The emergence of classroom L2 study feelings over interacting timescales. System 84, 14–23.

      Sampson, R.J. (2020a) The feeling classroom: Diversity of feelings in instructed L2 learning. Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching 14 (3), 203–217.

      Sampson, R.J. (2020b) Evolving understandings of practitioner action research from the inside. Manuscript in review.

      Sampson, R.J., Pinner, R.S., Falout, J. and Yashima, T. (2018) Simply researching complexity in language learning and teaching. In J. Mynard and I.K. Brady (eds) Stretching Boundaries: Papers from the Third International Psychology of Language Learning Conference, Tokyo (pp. 13–24). Tokyo: International Association for the Psychology of Language Learning.

      Ushioda, E. (2009) A person-in-context relational view of emergent motivation, self and identity. In E. Ushioda and Z. Dörnyei (eds) Motivation, Language Identity and the L2 Self (pp. 215–228). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.

      Vallacher, R.R. and Nowak, A. (2009) The dynamics of human experience: Fundamentals of dynamic social psychology. In S.J. Guastello, M. Koopmans and D. Pincus (eds) Chaos and Complexity in Psychology (pp. 370–401). New York: Cambridge University Press.

      Yashima, T., Ikeda, M. and Nakahira, S. (2016) Talk and silence in an EFL classroom: Interplay of learners and context. In J. King (ed.) The Dynamic Interplay between Context and the Learner (pp. 104–126.) Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.

      Yashima, T., MacIntyre, P. and Ikeda, M. (2018) Situated willingness to communicate in an L2: Interplay of individual characteristics and context. Language Teaching Research 22, 115–137.

      2

      Reflections on Researching Dynamics in Language Learning Psychology

      Peter D. MacIntyre, Sarah Mercer and Tammy Gregersen

      Introduction

      Humans are highly complex beings. Language and communication are complex systems. Combining psychology and language learning with the multifaceted communication process increases the complexity substantially. Add then to the mix the idea that all of these things are constantly changing over time and with even small differences in context, the challenge facing researchers in this field rounds into form. How does a research project capture such complexity and dynamism in a meaningful way without oversimplifying the lived reality? All three authors of this chapter have found taking a complexity-informed approach to research to be a most satisfying way of tackling this problem, but one that creates its share of challenges. In this chapter, we will reflect on our own experiences of grappling with the issues created by adopting a complexity perspective, focusing on timescales and dynamism. We will conclude this chapter with a series of recommendations, emerging from our experience, that we hope will assist other scholars wanting to research the dynamics of learner and/or teacher psychologies.

      A Gap between Theorizing and Empirical Research

      Within applied linguistics, theorizing complexity is well ahead of its empirical investigations (MacIntyre et al., 2017). Perhaps the reason for the imbalance between describing complex dynamic systems theory (CDST) and using it for empirical research is that CDST is itself meta-theoretical in nature, meaning that it is not a theory of language learning per se but an approach to creating theory (Larsen-Freeman, 2017); research does not test CDST directly. The ideas underlying CDST have been developed in several of the natural sciences, and now the social sciences and humanities including developmental psychology (Thelen & Smith, 1994) and communication (Fogel, 2006) have followed suit. In second language acquisition (SLA), CDST has attracted a collection of researchers interested in testing its applicability to various processes, many of whom are writing chapters in this volume and who have used it both as a theoretical lens as well as a basis for empirical design and analysis.

      SLA provides a rich context for understanding dynamics with its focus on language development, change and stability. However, it is not just language competences that are in a state of flux, but a range of related factors during these processes that are constantly interacting with learner and teacher psychologies. This makes the psychology of language learning an especially fertile ground for dynamic studies to take root. To date, research has tested the question of the applicability of CDST to familiar topics in this field such as willingness to communicate, motivation, the self, agency, anxiety, enjoyment, teacher efficacy and strategies; we discuss specific research examples. The studies we describe below unanimously conclude that the approach is well suited and the phenomena under study meet the key conditions for CDST. The work so far has indicated how a CDST perspective can lead to novel developments in conceptualization, methodology and data analysis/presentation; the possibilities for future research excite the imagination. The question now is how to proceed with research that will further our appreciation of the scope and potential of this theoretical framework to extend our understandings of the field of language learning and teaching.

      Benefits of a CDST Perspective on the Psychology of Language Learning

      Our interest in CDST is based in large part on what it offers as a way of thinking about the psychology of language learning (PLL) and communication and how CDST can contribute to new ways of studying the processes involved. The continuous interactions among the myriad of inter-personal and intra-personal processes are intricate, nuanced, contextualized and ever-changing. The focus on complex interactions requires a new set of tools for building theory and research.

      Before we consider the conceptual devices in the CDST toolbox that create the context for the approach, we must first establish what is being specified as a system. Defining a system is a matter of perspective; a lens that the researcher puts on a complex reality so that a system comes into focus as a functioning whole. The system remains open but its boundaries can be described. Discussions of CDST sometimes can become difficult to interpret if the system is not specified and this is an essential first step. Defining a system for study is a matter of emphasis, focussing on the most relevant processes. Not everything functions as a complex dynamic system and certain criteria must be fulfilled before a system can be defined (Mercer, 2016). To set the stage for the rest of the chapter and illustrate some of those characteristics, we highlight attributes of CDST that differ from other research approaches and make it especially relevant to PLL.

      Timescales

      Perhaps the most fundamental contribution of CDST is thinking explicitly about time and how it affects the processes under study. CDST forces a researcher to consider how a process unfolds over a chosen period of time. For example, in prior research, anxiety has typically been conceptualized as a long-term, trait-like quality of a learner, but in recent research it has been studied as rapidly changing from moment-to-moment, interacting with many of the features of communication events (MacIntyre, 2017). Both shorter and longer timescales have been used in prior research but they require very