Группа авторов

The Behavior of Animals


Скачать книгу

human personal relationships, and even in studies of religion and morality. Thus, while ethology as a set of concepts or as a theory of animal behavior has been largely superseded, the influence of its orienting attitudes has increased and is potent in other disciplines.

      While behavioral ecology took center stage in the study of animal behavior, many felt it to be impoverished by the neglect of problems of development and causation. This book will go a long way toward setting the balance straight. Each of the four problems is covered, and the chapters introduce the growing points in the study of animal behavior at the start of the twenty-first century.

      The idea for this book arose out of a need that we (and many of our colleagues) felt for a comprehensive textbook on animal behavior. There is no shortage of animal behavior textbooks, so why did we want to produce a new one? First, animal behavior is a dynamic field of research, and we believe that a modern textbook should incorporate all the contemporary subdisciplines of behavioral biology, such as animal welfare, evolutionary psychology, animal cognition, and behavioral neuroscience. In some ways, the science of animal behavior has become a victim of its own success, as it covers a much wider field than it did initially. Gone are the days when one author could write a textbook both comprehensive and authoritative: Robert Hinde’s classic Animal Behaviour: A Synthesis of Ethology and Comparative Psychology (1970) is an outstanding example of such a book, and it continues to inspire many of us. Given the breadth of contemporary animal behavior research, we felt that it was important to invite experts in the respective subdisciplines to write a chapter about their specialist topic.

      We are very pleased with the enthusiastic response we received from the authors invited to contribute to this book. They are all leaders in their respective fields, and we feel privileged that they participated in this project. Robert Hinde has passed away since his foreword was written, but his words are just as relevant to the second edition of this book as they were to the first. His influence remains.

      JOHAN J. BOLHUIS, LUC-ALAIN GIRALDEAU AND JERRY A. HOGAN

      INTRODUCTION

      The scientific study of animal behavior is often called ethology, a term used first by the nineteenth century French zoologist Isidore Geoffroy Saint Hilaire, but then used with its modern meaning by the American zoologist Wheeler (1902). Ethology is derived from the Greek ethos, meaning “character.” The word “ethics” is also derived from the same Greek word, which makes sense, because ethics is basically about how humans ought to behave. Unfortunately, the word “ethology” is also often confused with the word “ethnology” (the study of human peoples), with which it has nothing in common. In fact, the very word processor with which we are writing this chapter keeps prompting us to replace “ethology” by “ethnology”! For whatever the reason, the word “ethology” is not used as much as it used to be, although there is still an active animal behavior journal bearing this name. Instead of “ethology,” many authors now use the words “animal behavior” or “behavioral biology” when they refer to the scientific study of animal behavior.

      A Brief History of Behavioral Biology

      Early days

      Lorenz and Tinbergen

      In the middle of the twentieth century, the study of animal behavior became an independent scientific discipline, called ethology, mainly through the efforts of two biologists, the Austrian Konrad Lorenz (1903–1989) and the Dutchman Niko Tinbergen (1907–1988). It can be said that Lorenz was the more philosophical and theoretical of the two. He put forward a number of theoretical models on different aspects of animal behavior such as evolution and motivation. He was also the more outspoken of the two men, and some of his publications met with considerable controversy. Tinbergen was very much an experimentalist, who together with his students and collaborators conducted an extensive series of field and laboratory experiments on the behavior of animals of many different species. In 1973, Lorenz and Tinbergen were awarded the Nobel Prize for Physiology and Medicine. They shared their prize with Karl von Frisch (1886–1982), an Austrian comparative physiologist and ethologist who had pioneered research into the dance “language” of bees (Chapter 14).

      Ethology and comparative psychology