Группа авторов

The Behavior of Animals


Скачать книгу

information should be heavily influenced by its ecology. Neuroecology refers to the study of the neural mechanisms of behavior guided by functional and evolutionary principles. How do the evolutionary pressures for complex birdsong affect the evolution of the underlying neural substrate? How does having a large home range affect one’s ability to navigate? Does having to store food place selective pressure on spatial memory and its underlying brain regions? These functional and evolutionary approaches to the study of brain and cognition have come under considerable criticism from authors who claim that they are flawed, because Tinbergen’s four whys are being confounded (Bolhuis & Macphail 2001; Macphail & Bolhuis 2001; Bolhuis & Wynne 2009; Bolhuis 2015). For example, food storing in certain species of titmice and corvids has been interpreted as a case of adaptive specialization (Healy & Braithwaite 2000; Shettleworth 2010). According to this hypothesis, food-storing birds would have evolved superior spatial memory as well as a larger hippocampus (thought to be its neural substrate) compared to their nonstoring counterparts. Both parts of this hypothesis have not been confirmed by the data (see Chapter 8 for further discussion).

      Animal welfare and human nature

      Many people are interested in animal behavior out of mere curiosity, the need to know more about something. This is all very fine but there always comes a time when someone will ask “what is the purpose of studying animal behavior?” This question, whether from a research colleague, a friend, or a granting agency requires an answer expressed in terms of benefits to society. We see two areas in which animal behavior research can contribute to human society: animal welfare and understanding human nature.

      Animals are important contributors to wealth and quality of life. They provide us with nourishment, the means to find cures and treatments for our illnesses, as well as invaluable companionship. Almost all of the information contained in this book relies on experiments and research conducted with animals. There is a growing concern that animals used for human benefit, however, be exposed to as little unpleasantness as necessary. Are housing cages too small, or densities of individuals too high? Is the knowledge acquired from experiments sufficiently important to authorize animal experimentation? The answer to such, often difficult, questions depends in many ways on knowing something about an animal’s behavior (Chapter 10).

      SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS

      The study of the behavior of animals has grown into a highly diverse set of approaches and disciplines. Its subject area ranges from molecules and neurons to individuals and populations. One of Tinbergen’s major contributions to the study of animal behavior has been to make its goals explicit and clarify four types of questions that can be asked of behavior: causation, development, survival value, and evolution. In this book we strongly advocate Tinbergen’s position that behavior can only be understood through research on all four questions. In addition, we suggest that it should be made clear which of Tinbergen’s questions is addressed when a behavioral problem is investigated: a problem in one domain should not be investigated with concepts from another. The early chapters in this book examine primarily causal, mechanistic, and developmental questions, while the latter chapters examine survival value and evolution issues. But examples in many chapters also illustrate that multiple approaches are necessary for understanding a problem, affirming Tinbergen’s view.

      FURTHER READING

      REFERENCES

      1 Boakes, R.A. 1984. From Darwin to behaviorism. Psychology and the minds of animals. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

      2 Bolhuis, J.J. 2015. Evolution cannot explain how minds work. Behavioural Processes, 117, 82–91.

      3 Bolhuis, J.J. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (eds.). 2010. Animal behaviour, 4 volumes. London: Sage Publications.

      4 Bolhuis, J.J. & Macphail, E. M. 2001. A critique of the neuroecology of learning and memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 426–433.

      5 Bolhuis, J.J. & Macphail, E.M. 2002. Everything in neuroecology makes sense in the light of evolution. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 7–8.

      6 Bolhuis, J.J. & Verhulst, S. (Eds.) 2009. Tinbergen’s legacy: Function and mechanism in behavioral biology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

      7 Bolhuis, J.J. & Wynne, C.D.L. 2009. Can evolution explain how minds work? Nature, 458, 832–833.

      8 Broadbent, D.E. 1958. Perception and communication. London: Pergamon.

      9 Chomsky, N. 1959. A review of B.F. Skinner’s ‘Verbal Behavior’. Language, 35, 26–58.

      10 Darwin, C. 1859. On the Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, or the Preservation of Favoured Races in the Struggle for Life. London: John Murray.

      11 Davies, N.B., Krebs, J.R. & West, S.A. 2012. An introduction to behavioural ecology. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.

      12 Dewsbury, D. 1989. A brief history of the study of animal behavior in North America. In: P.P.G. Bateson & P.H. Klopfer (eds.), Perspectives in ethology, pp. 85–122. London: Plenum.

      13 Ewert, J.P. 1980. Neuroethology. An introduction to the neurophysiological fundamentals of behavior. Berlin: Springer.

      14 Healy, S. & Braithwaite, V. 2000. Cognitive ecology: a field of substance? Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 15, 22–26.

      15 Hogan, J.A. 1988. Cause and function in the development of behavior systems. In E. M. Blass (ed.), Handbook of behavioral neurobiology, vol. 9, pp. 63–106. New York: Plenum.

      16 Hogan, J.A. 1994. The concept of cause in the study of behavior. In J. A. Hogan & J. J. Bolhuis (eds.), Causal mechanisms of behavioural development, pp. 3–15. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

      17 Hogan,