figures, researchers have found that infants can perceive depth at birth (Slater, Rose, & Morison, 1984). Three- to 4-week-old infants blink their eyes when an object is moved toward their face as if to hit them, suggesting that they are sensitive to depth cues (Kayed, Farstad, & van der Meer, 2008; Náñez & Yonas, 1994). Infants learn about depth by observing and experiencing motion.
A classic series of studies using an apparatus called the visual cliff demonstrated that crawling influences how infants perceive depth. The visual cliff, as shown in Figure 4.14, is a Plexiglas-covered table bisected by a plank so that one side is shallow, with a checkerboard pattern right under the glass, and the other side is deep, with the checkerboard pattern a few feet below the glass (Gibson & Walk, 1960). In this classic study, crawling babies readily moved from the plank to the shallow side but not to the deep side, even if coaxed by their mothers, suggesting that they perceive the difference in depth (Walk, 1968). The more crawling experience infants have, the more likely they are to refuse to cross the deep side of the visual cliff (Bertenthal, Campos, & Barrett, 1984).
Figure 4.14 Visual Cliff
Three-month-old infants show a change in heart rate when placed face down on the glass surface of the deep side of the visual cliff, suggesting that they perceive depth, but do not fear it. Crawling babies, however, move to the shallow side of the visual cliff and refuse to cross the deep side of the visual cliff.
Source: Levine and Munsch (2010).
Does this mean that babies cannot distinguish the shallow and deep sides of the visual cliff until they crawl? No, because even 3-month-old infants who are too young to crawl distinguish shallow from deep drops. When placed face down on the glass surface of the deep side of the visual cliff, 3-month-old infants became quieter and showed a decrease in heart rate compared with when they were placed on the shallow side of the cliff (Dahl et al., 2013). The young infants can distinguish the difference between shallow and deep drops but do not yet associate fear with deep drops.
As infants gain experience crawling, their perception of depth changes. Newly walking infants avoid the cliff’s deep side even more consistently than do crawling infants (Dahl et al., 2013; Witherington, Campos, Anderson, Lejeune, & Seah, 2005). A new perspective on the visual cliff studies argues that infants avoid the deep side of the cliff not out of fear but simply because they perceive that they are unable to successfully navigate the drop; fear might be conditioned through later experiences, but infants are not naturally fearful of heights (Adolph, Kretch, & LoBue, 2014).
Hearing
The capacity to hear develops in the womb; in fact, hearing is the most well-developed sense at birth. Newborns are able to hear about as well as adults (Northern & Downs, 2014). Shortly after birth, neonates can discriminate among sounds, such as tones (Hernandez-Pavon, Sosa, Lutter, Maier, & Wakai, 2008). By 3 days of age, infants will turn their head and eyes in the general direction of a sound, and this ability to localize sound improves over the first 6 months (Clifton, Rochat, Robin, & Berthier, 1994; Litovsky & Ashmead, 1997).
As we will discuss in Chapter 5, the process of learning language begins at birth, through listening. Newborns are attentive to voices and can detect their mothers’ voices. Newborns only 1 day old prefer to hear speech sounds over similar-sounding nonspeech sounds (May, Gervain, Carreiras, & Werker, 2018). Newborns can perceive and discriminate nearly all sounds in human languages, but from birth, they prefer to hear their native language (Kisilevsky, 2016). Brain activity in the temporal and left frontal cortex in response to auditory stimuli indicates that newborns can discriminate speech patterns, such as differences in cadence among languages, suggesting an early developing neurological specialization for language (Gervain, Macagno, Cogoi, Peña, & Mehler, 2008; Gervain & Mehler, 2010).
Touch
Compared with vision and hearing, we know much less about the sense of touch in infants. In early infancy, touch, especially with the mouth, is a critical means of learning about the world (Piaget, 1936/1952). The mouth is the first part of the body to show sensitivity to touch prenatally and remains one of the most sensitive areas to touch after birth.
Touch, specifically a caregiver’s massage, can reduce stress responses in preterm and full-term neonates and is associated with weight gain in newborns (Álvarez et al., 2017). Skin-to-skin contact with a caregiver, as in kangaroo care (see Chapter 3), has an analgesic effect, reducing infants’ pain response to being stuck with a needle for vaccination (Pandita et al., 2018). Although it was once believed that newborns were too immature to feel pain, we now know that the capacity to feel pain develops even before birth. In one study, fetuses as early as 24 weeks of age observed with sophisticated ultrasound technology showed facial expressions suggesting distress or pain in response to a needle prick (Reissland, Francis, & Mason, 2013). The neonate’s capacity to feel pain has influenced debates about infant circumcision, as discussed in the Lives in Context feature.
Lives in Context: Family and Peer Context
Neonatal Circumcision
Neonatal circumcision, removal of the foreskin of the penis, is the oldest known planned surgery (Alanis & Lucidi, 2004). Although it is uncommon throughout much of the world, about three-quarters of males in the United States are circumcised (Morris et al., 2016). As shown in Figure 4.15, there are regional differences, with nearly twice as many infant circumcisions in the Midwest as in the West (Owings, Uddin, & Williams, 2013). In recent years, circumcision has come under increasing scrutiny within the United States as some charge that it places the newborn under great distress and confers few medical benefits.
For decades, many scientists and physicians believed that newborns did not feel pain, leading many to perform circumcision without pain management. We now know that even the fetus feels pain (Benatar & Benatar, 2003). Newborns show many indicators of distress during circumcision, such as a high-pitched wail, flailing, grimacing, and dramatic rises in heart rate, blood pressure, palm sweating, pupil dilation, muscle tension, and cortisol levels (Paix & Peterson, 2012). Analgesia (pain relief in which the newborn remains conscious) is safe and effective in reducing the pain associated with circumcision (AAP Task Force on Circumcision, 2012). Treatment as simple as administering a sugar solution to infants aids in pain management (Matsuda, 2017).
The medical benefits of circumcision are debated (Beal, 2017; Freedman, 2016). Benefits include reduced risk of having urinary tract infections, developing penile cancer, and acquiring HIV (AAP Task Force on Circumcision, 2012; American Medical Association, 1999; Morris et al., 2017). Some argue that these are relatively rare conditions and that the evidence regarding HIV transmission comes from research with adult males in Africa. Whether the same effects apply to infants in Western industrialized countries is uncertain (Alanis & Lucidi, 2004). Moreover, behavior is a more important factor in preventing HIV infection than is circumcision.
In 1999, both the American Medical Association and the American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) joined medical associations in Canada, Europe, and Australia in concluding that the benefits of circumcision are not large enough to recommend routine circumcision; instead, it is a parental decision. However, in 2012, the AAP modified its view to note that although it is a parental decision, the benefits of circumcision justify providing access to the procedure (by insurance companies) to families who choose it. Critical physicians and representatives of medical associations in Canada, Australia, and several European countries counter that the revised recommendation was not based on medical evidence but instead reflected cultural bias on the part of the AAP to support social practices common in the United States (Frisch et al., 2013).
Regardless, formal recommendations by medical associations may ultimately have little sway on parents (Freedman, 2016). Cultural traditions and religious factors influence