and can be used as speech information (Burnham et al., 2000; Han et al., 2018; Munhall & Vatikiotis‐Bateson, 2004). That visible mouth movements can inform about internal articulation may explain a striking recent finding. It turns out that, when observers are shown cross‐sectional ultrasound displays of internal tongue movements, they can readily integrate these novel displays with synchronized auditory speech information (D’Ausilio et al., 2014; see also Katz & Mehta, 2015).
The strong correspondences between auditory and visual speech information has allowed auditory speech to be synthesized based on tracking kinematic dimensions available on the face (e.g. Barker & Berthommier, 1999; Yehia, Kuratate, & Vatikiotis‐Bateson, 2002). Conversely, the correspondences have allowed facial animation to be effectively created based on direct acoustic signal parameters (e.g. Yamamoto, Nakamura, & Shikano, 1998). There is also evidence for surprisingly close correspondences between audible and visible macaque calls, which macaques can easily perceive as corresponding (Ghazanfar et al., 2005). This finding may suggest a traceable phylogeny of the supramodal basis for multisensory communication.
Importantly, there is evidence that perceivers make use of these crossmodal informational correspondences. While the supramodal thesis proposes that the relevant speech information takes a supramodal higher‐order form, the degree to which this information is simultaneously available in both modalities depends on a number of factors (e.g. visibility, audibility). The evidence shows that, in contexts for which the information is simultaneously available, perceivers take advantage of this correspondence (e.g. Grant & Seitz, 2000; Grant, 2001; Kim & Davis, 2004; Palmer & Ramsey, 2012; Schwartz, Berthommier, & Savariaux, 2004; Eskelund, Tuomainen, & Andersen, 2011; Rosen, Fourcin, & Moore, 1981). Research shows that the availability of segment‐to‐segment correspondence across the modalities’ information strongly predicts how well one modality will enhance the other (Grant & Seitz, 2000, 2001; Kim & Davis, 2004). Functionally, this finding supports the aforementioned “bimodal coherence‐masking protection” in that the informational correspondence across modalities allows one modality to boost the usability of the other (e.g. in the face of everyday masking degradation). In this sense, the supramodal thesis is consistent with the evidence supporting the bimodal coherence masking protection concept discussed earlier (Grant & Seitz, 2000; Grant, 2001; Kim & Davis, 2004). However, the supramodal thesis does go further by suggesting that: (1) the crossmodal correspondences are considered to be much more common and complex; and (2) the abstract form of information that can support correspondences is considered the primary type of information which the speech mechanism uses (regardless of the degree of moment‐to‐moment correspondence or specific availability of information in a modality).
General examples of supramodal information
While some progress has been made in identifying the detailed ways in which information takes the same specific form across modalities, more progress has been made to establish the general ways in which the informational forms are similar. In the previous version of this chapter, it was argued that both auditory and visual speech show an important primacy of time‐varying information (Rosenblum, 2005; see also Rosenblum, 2008). At the time that chapter was written, many descriptions of visual speech information were based on static facial feature information, and still images were often used as stimuli. Since then, most all methodological and conceptual interpretations of visual speech information have incorporated a critical dynamic component (e.g. Jesse & Bartoli, 2018; Jiang et al., 2007).
This contemporary emphasis on time‐varying information exists in both the behavioral and the neurophysiological research. A number of studies have examined how dynamic facial dimensions are extracted and stored for purposes of both phonetic and indexical perception (for a review, see Jesse & Bartoli, 2018). Other studies have shown that moment‐to‐moment visibility of articulator movements (as conveyed through discrete facial points) is highly predictive of lip‐reading performance (e.g. Jiang et al., 2007). These findings suggest that kinematic dimensions provide highly salient information for lip‐reading (Jiang et al., 2007). Other research has examined the neural mechanisms activated when perceiving dynamic speech information. For example, there is evidence that the mechanisms involved during perception of speech from isolated kinematic (point‐light) displays differ from those involved in recognizing speech from static faces (e.g. Santi et al., 2003). At the same time, brain reactivity to the isolated motion of point‐light speech does not qualitatively differ from reactivity to normal (fully illuminated) speaking faces (Bernstein et al., 2011). These neurophysiological findings are consistent with the primacy of time‐varying visible speech dimensions, which, in turn is analogous to the same primacy in audible speech (Rosenblum, 2005).
A second general way in which auditory and visual speech information takes a similar form is in how it interacts with – and informs about – indexical properties. As discussed in the earlier chapter, there is substantial research showing that both auditory and visual speech functions make use of talker information to facilitate phonetic perception (for reviews, see Nygaard, 2005; Rosenblum, 2005). It is easier to understand speech from familiar speakers (e.g. Borrie et al., 2013; Nygaard, 2005), and easier to lip‐read from familiar faces, even for observers who have no formal lip‐reading experience (e.g. Lander & Davies, 2008; Schweinberger & Soukup, 1998; Yakel, Rosenblum, & Fortier, 2000).
In these talker‐facilitation effects, it could be that an observer’s phonetic perception is facilitated by their familiarity with the separate vocal and facial characteristics provided by each modality. However, research conducted in our lab suggests that perceivers may also gain experience with the deeper, supramodal talker dimensions available across modalities (Rosenblum, Miller, & Sanchez, 2007; Sanchez, Dias, & Rosenblum, 2013). Our research shows that the talker experience gained through one modality can be shared across modalities to facilitate phonetic perception in the other. For example, becoming familiar with a talker by lip‐reading them (without sound) for one hour allows a perceiver to then better understand that talker’s auditory speech (Rosenblum, Miller, & Sanchez, 2007). Conversely, listening to the speech of a talker for one hour allows a perceiver to better lip‐read from that talker (Sanchez, Dias, & Rosenblum, 2013). Interestingly, this crossmodal talker facilitation works for both old words (perceived during familiarization) and new words, suggesting that the familiarity is not contained in specific lexical representations (Sanchez, Dias, & Rosenblum, 2013). Instead, the learned supramodal dimensions may be based on talker‐specific phonetic information contained in the idiolect of the perceived talker (e.g. Remez, Fellowes, & Rubin, 1997; Rosenblum et al., 2002).
This interpretation can also explain our finding that learning to identify talkers can be shared across modalities (Simmons et al., 2015). In this demonstration, idiolectic information was isolated visually through a point‐light technique, and audibly through sinewave resynthesis (e.g. Remez et al. 1997; Rosenblum, Yakel, & Baseer, 2002). With these methods we observed that experience of learning to recognize talkers through point‐light displays transfers to allow better recognition of the same speakers heard in sinewave sentences. No doubt, our findings are related to past observations that perceivers can match a talker’s voice and speaking face, even when both signals are rendered as isolated phonetic information (sinewave speech and point‐light speech; Lachs & Pisoni, 2004). In all of these examples, perceivers may be learning the particular idiolectic properties of talkers’ articulation, which can be informed by both auditory and visual speech information.
We have termed this interpretation of these findings the supramodal learning hypothesis. The hypothesis simply argues that part of what the speech function learns through experience is the supramodal properties related to a talker’s articulation. Because these articulatory properties are distal in nature, experience with learning in one modality can be shared across modalities to support crossmodal talker facilitation, learning, and matching.
We further argue that the supramodal learning hypothesis helps explain bimodal training benefits recently reported in the literature. Bimodal training benefits occur when an observer is able to better understand degraded auditory speech after first being trained with congruent visual speech added to the degraded signal (Bernstein