Группа авторов

The Handbook of Speech Perception


Скачать книгу

2014; Eberhardt, Auer, & Bernstein, 2014; Kawase et al., 2009; Lidestam et al., 2014; Moradi et al., 2019; Pilling & Thomas, 2011; but see Wayne & Johnsrude, 2012). For example, vocoded auditory speech is easier to understand on its own if a perceiver is first trained to listen to vocoded speech while seeing congruent visual speech. Bimodal training effects are also known to facilitate: (1) talker recognition from auditory speech (Schall & von Kriegstein, 2014; Schelinski, Riedel, & von Kriegstein, 2014; Sheffert et al., 2002; von Kriegstein et al., 2008; von Kriegstein & Giraud, 2006); (2) talker‐familiarity effects; (3) language development (Teinonen et al., 2008); and (4) second‐language learning (Hardison, 2005; Hazan et al., 2005). (There are also many examples of bimodal training benefits outside of speech perception suggesting that it may be a general learning strategy of the brain (e.g. Shams et al., 2011).)

      Importantly, neurophysiological correlates to many of these effects have revealed mechanisms that can be modulated with bimodal learning. It has long been known that the pSTS responds when observers are asked to report the speech they either see or hear. More recent research suggests that this activation is enhanced when a perceiver sees or hears a talker with whom they have some audiovisual experience (von Kriegstein & Giraud, 2006; von Kriegstein et al., 2005). If observers are tasked, instead, with identifying the voice of a talker, they show activation in an area associated with face recognition (fusiform face area; von Kriegstein et al., 2008; von Kriegstein & Giraud, 2006). This activation will also be enhanced by prior audiovisual exposure to the talker (von Kriegstein & Giraud, 2006). These findings are consistent with the possibility that observers are learning talker‐specific articulatory properties, as the supramodal learning hypothesis suggests.

      Other theories have arisen to explain these bimodal training benefits including (1) tacit recruitment of the associated face dimensions when later listening to the auditory‐alone speech (Riedel et al., 2015; Schelinski, Riedel, & von Kriegstein, 2014); and (2) greater access to auditory primitives based on previously experienced associations with the visual speech component (Bernstein et al., 2013; Bernstein, Eberhardt, & Auer, 2014). However, both of these theories are based on a mechanism that requires experiencing associations between concurrent audio and visual streams in order to improve subsequent audio‐alone speech perception. Recall, however, that our crossmodal talker‐facilitation findings (Rosenblum et al., 2007) show that such bimodal experience is not necessary for later auditory speech facilitation. Accordingly, we argue that at least some component of bimodal training benefits is based on both modalities providing common talker‐specific talker information. To examine this possibility, our laboratory is currently testing whether a bimodal training benefit can, in fact, occur without the associations afforded by concurrent audio and visual speech information, as the supramodal learning hypothesis would predict.

      In sum, as we argued in our 2005 chapter, both auditory and visual speech share the general informational commonalities of being composed of time‐varying information which is intimately tied to indexical information. However, since 2005, another category of informational commonality can be added to this list: information in both streams can act to guide the indexical details of a production response. It is well known that during live conversation each participant’s productions are influenced by the indexical details of the speech they have just heard (e.g. Pardo, 2006; Pardo et al., 2013; for a review, see Pardo et al., 2017). This phonetic convergence shows that interlocuters’ utterances often subtly mimic aspects of the utterances of the person with whom they are speaking. This phenomenon occurs not only during live interaction, but also when subjects are asked to listen to recorded words and to say each word out loud. There have been many explanations for this phenomenon, including that it helps facilitate the interaction socially (e.g. Pardo et al., 2012). Phonetic convergence may also reveal the tacit connection between speech perception and production, as if the two function share a “common currency” (e.g. Fowler, 2004).

      Importantly, recent research from our lab and others suggests that phonetic convergence is not an alignment toward an interlocuter’s sound of speech as much as toward their articulatory style – conveyed supramodally. We have shown that, despite having no formal lip‐reading experience, perceivers will produce words containing the indexical properties of words they have just lip‐read (Miller, Sanchez, & Rosenblum, 2010). Further, the degree to which talkers converge toward lip‐read words is comparable to that observed for convergence to heard words. Other research from our lab shows that, during live interactions, seeing an interlocuter increases the degree of convergence over simply hearing them (Dias & Rosenblum, 2011), and that this increase is based on the availability of visible speech articulation (Dias & Rosenblum, 2016). Finally, it seems that the visual information for articulatory features (voice‐onset time) can integrate with auditory information to shape convergence (Sanchez, Miller, & Rosenblum, 2010). This finding also suggests that the streams are merged by the time they influence a spontaneous production response.

      There is currently a debate on whether the involvement of motor areas is necessary for audiovisual integration and for speech perception, in general (for a review, see Rosenblum, Dorsi, & Dias, 2016). But it is clear that the speech system treats auditory and visual speech information similarly for priming phonetic convergence in production responses. Thus, phonetic convergence joins the characteristics of critical time‐varying and indexical dimensions as an example of general informational commonality across audio and video streams. In this sense, the recent phonetic convergence research supports a supramodal perspective.

      Research on multisensory speech has flourished since 2005. This research has spearheaded a revolution in our understanding of the perceptual brain. The brain