Группа авторов

The Handbook of Speech Perception


Скачать книгу

a phenomenon that is frequently assumed but is not strongly supported by actual data – babbling drift. The hypothesis that the sounds of babbling drift over time was first proposed by Roger Brown (1958). Brown suggested that the phonetic repertoire in the babbling of infants slowly begins to resemble the phonetics of the language environment that they are exposed to and begins to not include sounds that are absent from the native language. As the review by Best et al. (2016) indicates, the support for this idea is mixed, particularly from transcription studies and perceptual studies where naive listeners attempted to identify the language environment of the infant’s babbling.

Schematic illustration of average F1 (circles) and F2 (triangles) frequencies estimates across time for adults (top panel), young children (middle panel), and toddlers (bottom panel).

      Source: MacDonald et al., © 2012, Elsevier.

      Another approach has been to use recordings of infants babbling as perceptual stimuli and ask adult listeners to categorize what native language the infants have. These studies have also shown mixed results, with some experiments reporting that listeners can discriminate the home language of the infants (e.g. de Boysson‐Bardies, Sagart, & Durand, 1984) while others showed no perceptual difference (e.g. Thevenin et al., 1985). The more serious concern about these studies is that listeners were likely tuning into prosodic differences in the babbling rather than the segmental differences that would be predicted by babbling drift. The ability to perceptually distinguish the language of babbling has been shown for low‐pass filtered stimuli (e.g. Whalen, Levitt, & Wang, 1991) and this supports the idea that it is prosodic differences that are driving these results. A recent controlled study (Lee et al., 2017) with a large number of stimuli found that perceptual categorizations of Chinese‐ and English‐learning babies’ utterances at 8, 10, and 12 months of age were only reliable for a small subset of the stimuli (words or canonical syllables that resembled words). These effects were modest and suggest that early lexical development rather than babbling may be where the home language shows its earliest influence.

      Direct measurements of babbling acoustics have shown evidence for babbling drift, albeit only small effects. For example, Whalen, Levitt, and Goldstein (2007) measured voice onset time (VOT) in French‐ and English‐learning infants at ages 9 and 12 months. There were no differences in VOT or in the duration of prevoicing that was observed. However, there was a greater incidence of prevoicing in the French babies which corresponds to adult French–English differences.

      This puzzle reflects the general problem of understanding the relationship between the processes of listening to speech and producing it. Liberman (1996, p. 247) stated:

      In all communication, sender and receiver must be bound by a common understanding about what counts; what counts for the sender must count for the receiver, else communication does not occur. Moreover, the processes of production and perception must somehow be linked; their representation must, at some point, be the same.

      This is certainly true in a very general sense but the roles played in communication by the auditory signal that reaches the listener and by the signal that reaches the speaker are dramatically different. For the listener, the signal is involved in categorical discrimination and information transmission, while for the talker the signal is primarily thought to influence motor precision and error correction. These two issues are not independent but are far from equivalent. The problem for researchers is that the perception and production of speech are so intrinsically intertwined in communication that it is difficult to distinguish the influence of these “two solitudes” of speech research on spoken language.

      While historically the relationship between speech perception and production has been implicated as explanations of language change, patterns of language disorder, and the developmental time course of speech acquisition, there has been little comprehensive theorizing about how speech input and output interact (Levelt, 2013). Recently, Kittredge and Dell (2016) outlined three stark hypotheses about the relationship between speech perception and production. In their view, the representations for perception and production could be completely separate, absolutely inseparable, or separable under some if not many conditions.

      A number of different types of experimental evidence might distinguish these possibilities, including (1) data that examine whether learning/adaptation changes in perception influence production and vice versa; (2) correlational data showing individual differences in the processing of speech perception and production (e.g. perceptual precision and production variability); and (3) data showing interference between the two processes of perception and production.

       Learning/adaptation changes