Группа авторов

The Handbook of Language and Speech Disorders


Скачать книгу

researchers have discussed this issue across many of the communicative and cognitive exceptionalities (Gibbs & Elliott, 2015; Gipps, 1999; Glaser & Silver, 1994; Klassen, Tze, Betts, & Gordon, 2011; Messick, 1984; Rogers, 2002; Rolison & Medway, 1985; Skrtic, 1991; Sleeter, 1996).

      Positive impact, however, goes even further with services provided. An appropriate label does not just create reactionary influences to provide service delivery; it also enables a discerning clinician to carefully prepare a treatment plan that is tailored to the needs of the individual now accurately identified and labeled. In doing so, having the valid diagnostic label may lead to specific intervention that will overcome the identified deficits (Archer & Green, 1996; Brinton & Fujiki, 2010; Gross, 1994; Kamhi, 2014; Müller, Cannon, Kornblum, Clark, & Powers, 2016). In effect, strong assessment resulting in an accurate diagnosis is essential for good intervention to occur. To use a metaphor from Brinton and Fujiki (2010), “you must know where you are going to plan your route.”

       1.4.2 Negative Consequences

      Labels, however, may also have negative effects. This is particularly true if the applied labels are not valid, or if a valid label is inappropriately or incorrectly applied. The most obvious destructive consequence occurs when an inaccurate label is applied. There are two ways that this may happen. For instance, a schoolchild may exhibit communicative or academic difficulties that are not due to actual impairment, but is misdiagnosed and labeled as disordered. In such a case, the mislabeled individual may be placed in special education or other remedial program. Often this means that the curriculum is reduced so that more time and effort may be spent on content that is deemed most important and salient, or that specific learning strategies are employed that may be necessary for impaired learners, but that limit learning by average students (Grigorenko, 2009; Van Kraayenoord, 2010). In these cases, inappropriate labeling provides poor opportunities for normal learners, and the expectations directed toward the inappropriately labeled individuals are reduced (Brantlinger, 1997; Connor & Ferri, 2005; Frattura & Capper, 2006; Rogers, 2002). Such situations often arise in contexts where students have language or learning difficulties arising out of cultural or language differences. When such students are referred for assessment, they are often mislabeled (Artiles & Ortiz, 2002; Cummins, 2000; Hamayan, Marler, Sanchez‐Lopez, & Damico, 2013; Trueba, 1988; Wilkinson & Ortiz, 1986). Their difficulties due to differences are categorized as disorders, and they are placed inappropriately in special education (Connor, 2006; Hamayan et al., 2013; Magnuson & Waldfogel, 2005; Trent, Artiles, & Englert, 1998).

      The second type of misdiagnosis occurs when an individual with a difficulty due to some actual impairment is identified as having a different impairment. In these instances, the genuine impairment is not adequately addressed, remedial plans and the expectations for improvement may be inappropriate, and little positive change occurs. Labeling is particularly problematic in these cases due to the tendency to attach a stereotype to a label, and then to focus on the stereotypic behaviors in the labeled individuals regardless of the presence of other, even conflicting, symptoms (Madon, Hilbert, Kyriakatos, & Vogel, 2006).

      While the most obvious harmful consequences may result from errors in labeling, the negative impact of the process is more complex and insidious. It must be remembered that labels are actually summaries of complex symptoms, a “mental shorthand” that plays into the human inclination to stereotype and make generalizations (Leyens et al., 1994). While this propensity does assist in communicating ideas underlying the labels, there is also the tendency to stop looking at the individual and start assuming that he or she is defined by the label and its characteristics. This assumption enables the professional to stereotype the unique aspects of the labeled individual so that all children with the same label are considered similar. This results in a failure to notice and account for personal strengths and difficulties. The consequence is a reduction of individual differences and a limitation on the ways in which the individual is perceived and treated (Lubinski, 2000; Madon et al., 2006).

      Another negative consequence of labeling arises from the very practice of assigning a label. If the intent is to label an individual, then often there is an assumption that not only the symptoms, but their origins exist within the individual being labeled. Consequently, there is a predisposition to localize the problems within the individual rather than to search for multiple factors and extraneous variables, including, for example, teaching styles, prior exposure to opportunities to learn and apply the targeted skills, and diversity issues in schoolchildren (Brown, 1995; Coles, 1987; Conrad, 2000; Forness, 1976; McDermott, 1987, 1993; Rapley, 2004). The decision to focus on intrinsic causal factors rather than extrinsic factors (or at least a combination) is likely a primary reason for the overrepresentation of various ethnic and socioeconomic groups in some aspects of special education (e.g., Cummins, 2000; Damico, 1991; Hamayan et al., 2013; Hood, McDermott, & Cole, 1980; McDermott & Varenne, 1995; McNamara, 1998). Treating labels as verification of intrinsic disability may also be based on the assumption that the source of all educational difficulties is related to causes that are intrinsic to students (Carroll, 1997; Gutkin & Nemeth, 1997). This assumption is exacerbated by the general lack of familiarity that the public has with the principles of language and learning in academic and communicative contexts.

      Unfortunately, this belief in the primacy of intrinsic causal factors, exacerbated by labeling, frequently results in negative consequences. The label is often used to “explain away” the problem, so that if, for example, a child experiences poor teaching or unresponsive therapy, the propensity is to place blame on the child, not the methods or the teacher/therapist. The within‐child deficit model makes for an easy and effective excuse.