40–52.
79 Kubinyi, E. and Iotchev, I.B. (2020). A preliminary study toward a rapid assessment of age‐related behavioral differences in family dogs. Animals 10: 1222.
80 Landsberg, G.M., Hunthasuen, W.L., and Ackerman, L.J. (2003). The effects of aging on the behavior of senior pets. In: Handbook of Behavior Problems of the Dog and Cat, 2nd ed. (eds. G.M. Landsberg, W.L. Hunthausen, and L. Ackerman), 269–304. Edinburgh: Elsevier.
81 Leaver, S.D.A. and Reimchen, T.E. (2008). Behavioural responses of Canis familiaris to different tail lengths of a remotely‐controlled life‐size dog replica. Behaviour 145: 377–390.
82 Lisberg, A.E. and Snowdon, C.T. (2009). The effects of sex, gonadectomy and status on investigation patterns of unfamiliar conspecific urine in domestic dogs, Canis familiaris. Anim. Behav. 77: 1147–1154.
83 Loftus, B.A., Asher, L., and Casey, R.A. (2018). Sequential analysis to quantify variability in canine abnormal repetitive behaviour. Proceedings of the 11th International Conference on Methods and Techniques in Behavioral Research, Manchester, United Kingdom (6–8 June 2018). Manchester, UK: Measuring Behavior 2018.
84 Lord, K., Feinstein, M., and Coppinger, R. (2009). Barking and mobbing. Behav. Process. 81: 358–368.
85 MacLean, E. L., Snyder‐Mackler, N., vonHoldt, B. M. et al. (2019). Highly heritable and functionally relevant breed differences in dog behaviour. Proc. R. Soc. B. 286: 20190716.
86 Mariti, C., Carlone, B., Ricci, E. et al. (2014). Intraspecific attachment in adult domestic dogs (Canis familiaris): Preliminary results. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 152: 64–72.
87 Mariti, C., Carlone, B., Votta, E. et al. (2017). Intraspecific relationships in adult domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) living in the same household: A comparison of the relationship with the mother and an unrelated older female dog. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 194: 62–66.
88 Mariti, C., Gazzano, A., Moore, J.L. et al. (2012). Perception of dogs’ stress by their owners. J. Vet. Behav. 7: 213–219.
89 Marshall‐Pescini, S., Cafazzo, S., Virányi, Z. et al. (2017). Integrating social ecology in explanations of wolf‐dog behavioral differences. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 16: 80–86.
90 Mason, G.J. (1991). Stereotypies: A critical review. Anim. Behav. 41: 1015–1037.
91 Mason, G.J. and Latham, N.R. (2004). Can't stop, won't stop: Is stereotypy a reliable animal welfare indicator? Anim. Welf. 13: S57–S69.
92 McGowan, R.T.S., Rehn, T., Norling, Y. et al. (2014). Positive affect and learning: Exploring the “Eureka Effect” in dogs. Anim. Cog. 17: 577–587.
93 McGuire, B., Olsen, B., Bemis, K.E. et al. (2018). Urine marking in male domestic dogs: Honest or dishonest? J. Zool. 306: 163–170.
94 McMillan, F.D. (2017). Behavioral and psychological outcomes for dogs sold as puppies through pet stores and/or born in commercial breeding establishments: Current knowledge and putative causes. J. Vet. Behav. 19: 14–26.
95 Mehrkam, L.R and Wynne, C.D.L. (2014). Behavioral differences among breeds of domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris): Current status of the science. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 155: 12–27.
96 Mertens, P.A. and Unshelm, J. (1996). Effects of group and individual housing on the behavior of kennelled dogs in animal shelters. Anthrozoös 9: 40–51.
97 Miklósi, Á. (2015). Affiliative and agonistic social relationships. In: Dog Behaviour, Evolution and Cognition, 2nd ed., 223–251. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
98 Miklósi, Á., Polgárdi, R., Topál, J. et al. (2000). Intentional behaviour in dog‐human communication: An experimental analysis of “showing” behaviour in the dog. Anim. Cog. 3: 159–166.
99 Mills, K.E., Robbins, J., and von Keyserlingk, M.A.G. (2016). Tail docking and ear cropping dogs: Public awareness and perceptions. PLOS ONE 11: e0158131.
100 Mongillo, P., Pitteri, E., Carnier, P. et al. (2013). Does the attachment system towards owners change in aged dogs? Physiol. Behav. 120: 64–69.
101 Müller, C.A., Schmitt, K., Barber, A.L.A., et al. (2015). Dogs can discriminate emotional expressions of human faces. Curr. Biol. 25: 601–605.
102 Neuhaus, V.W. (1981). The importance of sniffing to the olfaction of the dog. Z. Saugetierkd. 46: 301–310.
103 Newman, J., Christley, R.M., Westgarth, C. (2017). Risk factors for dog bites—An epidemiological perspective. In: Dog Bites: A Multidisciplinary Perspective (eds. D.S. Mills and C. Westgarth), 133–158. Sheffield, UK: 5m Publishing.
104 Owczarczak‐Garstecka, S.C., Watkins, F., Christley, R. et al. (2018). Online videos indicate human and dog behaviour preceding dog bites and the context in which bites occur. Sci. Rep. 8: 7147.
105 Pierantoni, L., Albertini, M., and Pirrone, F. (2011). Prevalence of owner‐reported behaviours in dogs separated from the litter at two different ages. Vet. Rec. 169: 468–473.
106 Pilley, J. W. and Reid, A. K. (2011). Border collie comprehends object names as verbal referents. Behav. Processes 86: 184–195.
107 Pluijmakers, J.J.T.M., Appleby, D.L., and Bradshaw, J.W.S. (2010). Exposure to video images between 3 and 5 weeks of age decreases neophobia in domestic dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 126: 51–58.
108 Polgár, Z., Blackwell, E.J., and Rooney, N.J. (2019). Assessing the welfare of kennelled dogs—A review of animal‐based measures. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 213: 1–13.
109 Pongrácz, P., Czinege, N., Haynes, T.M.P. et al. (2016). The communicative relevance of auditory nuisance barks that are connected to negative inner states in dogs can predict annoyance level in humans. Interaction 17: 19–40.
110 Pongrácz, P., Molnár, C., and Miklósi, Á. (2006). Acoustic parameters of dog barks carry emotional information for humans. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 100: 228–240.
111 Powell, L., Edwards, K.M., Bauman, A. et al (2019a). Canine endogenous oxytocin responses to dog‐walking and affiliative human‐dog interactions. Animals 9: 51.
112 Powell, L., Guastella, A.J., McGreevy, P. et al. (2019b). The physiological function of oxytocin in humans and its acute response to human‐dog interactions: A review of the literature. J. Vet. Behav. 30: 25–32.
113 Protopopova, A. (2016). Effects of sheltering on physiology, immune function, behavior, and the welfare of dogs. Physiol. Behav. 159: 95–103.
114 Protopopova, A. and Wynne, C.D.L. (2015). Improving in‐kennel presentation of shelter dogs through response‐dependent and response‐independent treat delivery. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 48: 590–601.
115 Quaranta, A., Sinischalchi, M., and Vallortigara, G. (2007). Asymmetric tail‐wagging responses by dogs to different emotive stimuli. Curr. Biol. 17: R199–R201.
116 Quignon, P., Kirkness, E., Cadieu, E. et al. (2003). Comparison of the canine and human olfactory receptor gene repertoires. Genome Biol. 4: 80.1–80.9.
117 Ramos, D. and Mills, D.S. (2019). Limitations in the learning of verbal content by dogs during the training of OBJECT and ACTION commands. J. Vet. Behav. 31: 92–99.
118 Rehn, T. and Keeling, L.J. (2016). Measuring dog‐owner relationships: Crossing boundaries between animal behaviour and human psychology. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 183: 1–9.
119 Reid, P.J. (2009). Adapting to the human world: Dogs’ responsiveness to our social cues. Behav. Process. 80: 325–333.
120 Reisner, I.R., Nance, M.L., Zeller, J.S. et al. (2011). Behavioural characteristics associated with dog bites to children presenting to an urban trauma centre. Inj. Prev. 17: 348–353.
121 Riedel, J., Schumann, K., Kaminski, J. et al. (2008). The early ontogeny of human‐dog communication. Anim. Behav. 75: 1003–1014.
122 Romero, T., Nagasawa, M., Mogi, K. et al. (2014). Oxytocin promotes social bonding in dogs. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111: 9085–9090.
123 Rooney, N.J. and Bradshaw, J.W.S. (2014). Canine